In the scientific study of vision, smooth pursuit describes a type of eye movement in which the eyes remain fixated on a moving object. It is one of two ways that visual animals can voluntarily shift gaze, the other being saccadic eye movements. Pursuit differs from the vestibulo-ocular reflex, which only occurs during movements of the head and serves to stabilize gaze on a stationary object. Most people are unable to initiate pursuit without a moving visual signal. The pursuit of _targets moving with velocities of greater than 30°/s tends to require catch-up saccades. Smooth pursuit is asymmetric: most humans and primates tend to be better at horizontal than vertical smooth pursuit, as defined by their ability to pursue smoothly without making catch-up saccades. Most humans are also better at downward than upward pursuit.[1] Pursuit is modified by ongoing visual feedback.
Measurement
editThere are two basic methods for recording smooth pursuit eye movements, and eye movement in general. The first is with a search coil. This technique is most common in primate research, and is extremely accurate. An eye movement shifts the orientation of the coil to induce an electric current, which is translated into horizontal and vertical eye position. The second technique is an eye tracker. This device, while somewhat more noisy, is non-invasive and is often used in human psychophysics and recently also in instructional psychology. It relies on the infrared illumination of the pupil to track eye position with a camera.[2]
During oculomotor experiments, it is often important to ensure that no saccades occurred when the subject was supposed to be smoothly pursuing a _target. Such eye movements are called catch-up saccades and are more common when pursuing at high speeds. Researchers are able to discard portions of eye movement recordings that contain saccades, in order to analyze the two components separately. Saccadic eye movements differ from the smooth pursuit component by their very high initial acceleration and deceleration, and peak velocity.[3]
Neural circuitry
editThe neural circuitry underlying smooth pursuit is an object of debate. The first step towards the initiation of pursuit is to see a moving _target. Signals from the retina ascend through the lateral geniculate nucleus and activate neurons in primary visual cortex. Primary visual cortex sends the information about the _target to the middle temporal visual cortex, which responds very selectively to directions of movement. The processing of motion in this area is necessary for smooth pursuit responses.[4] This sensory area provides the motion signal, which may or may not be smoothly pursued. A region of cortex in the frontal lobe, known as the frontal pursuit area, responds to particular vectors of pursuit, and can be electrically stimulated to induce pursuit movements.[5] Recent evidence suggests that the superior colliculus also responds during smooth pursuit eye movement.[6] These two areas are likely involved in providing the "go"-signal to initiate pursuit, as well as selecting which _target to track. The "go"-signal from the cortex and the superior colliculus is relayed to several pontine nuclei, including the dorsolateral pontine nuclei and the nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis.[7] The neurons of the pons are tuned to eye velocity and are directionally selective, and can be stimulated to change the velocity of pursuit. The pontine nuclei project to the cerebellum, specifically the vermis and the paraflocculus. These neurons code for the _target velocity and are responsible for the particular velocity profile of pursuit.[citation needed] The cerebellum, especially the vestibulo-cerebellum, is also involved in the online correction of velocity during pursuit.[8] The cerebellum then projects to optic motoneurons, which control the eye muscles and cause the eye to move.
Stages of smooth pursuit
editPursuit eye movement can be divided into two stages: open-loop pursuit and closed-loop pursuit. Open-loop pursuit is the visual system's first response to a moving object we want to track and typically lasts ~100 ms. Therefore, this stage is ballistic: Visual signals have not yet had time to correct the ongoing pursuit velocity or direction.[9] The second stage of pursuit, closed-loop pursuit, lasts until the pursuit movement has ceased. This stage is characterized by the online correction of pursuit velocity to compensate for retinal slip. In other words, the pursuit system tries to null retinal velocity of the object of interest. This is achieved at the end of the open-loop phase. In the closed-loop phase, the eye angular velocity and _target angular velocity are nearly equal.
Smooth pursuit and spatial attention
editVarious lines of research suggests a tight coupling for closed loop pursuit and spatial attention. For instance, during the close loop phase selective attention is coupled to the pursuit _target such that untracked _targets which move in the same direction with the _target are poorly processed by the visual system.[10] Recently, a loose coupling of open loop pursuit and attention was suggested, when there is only one possible moving _target.[11] This difference between pursuit and saccades may be accounted for by the differences in latency. Pursuit eye movements are initiated within 90-150 ms, while typical latencies for voluntary saccades are in the order of 200-250 ms [12]
Smooth pursuit in the absence of a visual _target
editPerforming smooth pursuit without a moving visual stimulus is difficult,[13] and typically results in a series of saccades. However, pursuit without a visible _target is possible under some particular conditions, that show the importance of high-level functions in smooth pursuit maintenance.
If you know which way a _target will move, or know the _target trajectory (because it is periodic for instance), you can initiate pursuit before the _target motion actually starts, especially if you know exactly when the motion will start.[12][14] It is also possible to maintain pursuit if a _target momentarily disappears, especially if the _target appears to be occluded by a larger object.[14]
Under conditions in which there is no visual stimulation (in total darkness), we can still perform smooth pursuit eye movements with the help of a proprioceptive motion signal (e.g. your moving finger).[15]
Following stimuli from peripheral gaze
editWhen a bright light appears in the periphery, the fastest it can achieve a smooth pursuit is 30°/second. It first fixes the gaze to the peripheral light, and if not in excess of 30°/second, will follow the _target equally with the movement. At higher velocities, the eye will not move smoothly, and requires corrective saccades. Unlike saccades, this process uses a continuous feedback system, which is based strictly on error.[16]
Distinction between smooth pursuit, optokinetic nystagmus, and ocular following response
editAlthough we can clearly separate smooth pursuit from the vestibulo-ocular reflex, we can not always draw a clear separation between smooth pursuit and other tracking eye movements like the slow phase of the optokinetic nystagmus and the ocular following response (OFR), discovered in 1986 by Miles, Kawano, and Optican,[17] which is a transient ocular tracking response to full-field motion. The latter are both slow eye movements in response to extended _targets, with the purpose of stabilizing the image. Therefore, some processing stages are shared with the smooth pursuit system.[18] Those different kinds of eye movements may not be simply differentiated by the stimulus that is appropriate to generate them, as smooth pursuit eye movements can be generated to track extended _targets as well. The main difference may lie in the voluntary nature of pursuit eye movements.[19]
Smooth-pursuit deficits
editSmooth pursuit requires the coordination of many brain regions that are far away from each other. This makes it particularly susceptible to impairment from a variety of disorders and conditions.[citation needed]
Schizophrenia
editThere is significant evidence that smooth pursuit is deficient in people with schizophrenia and their relatives. People with schizophrenia tend to have trouble pursuing very fast _targets. This impairment is correlated with less activation in areas known to play a role in pursuit, such as the frontal eye field.[20] However, other studies have shown that people with schizophrenia show relatively normal pursuit, compared to controls, when tracking objects that move unexpectedly. The greatest deficits are when the patients track objects of a predictable velocity which begin moving at a predictable time.[21] This study speculates that smooth pursuit deficits in schizophrenia are a function of the patients' inability to store motion vectors.
Autism
editPeople with autism show a plethora of visual deficits. One such deficit is to smooth pursuit. Children with autism show reduced velocity of smooth pursuit compared to controls during ongoing tracking.[22] However, the latency of the pursuit response is similar to controls. This deficit appears to only emerge after middle adolescence.
Trauma
editPeople with post traumatic stress disorder, with secondary psychotic symptoms, show pursuit deficits.[23] These patients tend to have trouble maintaining pursuit velocity above 30 degree/second. A correlation has also been found between performance on tracking tasks and a childhood history of physical and emotional abuse.[24]
Drugs and Alcohol
edit"Lack of Smooth Pursuit" is a scorable clue on the NHTSA's standardized field sobriety tests. The clue, in combination with others, may be used to determine if a person is impaired by alcohol and/or drugs. Drugs causing lack of smooth pursuit include depressants, some inhalants, and dissociative anesthetics (such as phencyclidine or ketamine).[citation needed]
Preterm Birth
editChildren born very preterm show smooth pursuit deficits compared to paired controls born at full term.[25] This delay in smooth pursuit has also been linked to later neurodevelopment in toddlerhood in children born very preterm.[26]
See also
editReferences
edit- ^ Grasse, K. L; Lisberger, S. G (1992). "Analysis of a naturally occurring asymmetry in vertical smooth pursuit eye movements in a monkey". Journal of Neurophysiology. 67 (1): 164–79. doi:10.1152/jn.1992.67.1.164. PMID 1552317.
- ^ "What is Eye Tracking and How Does it Work? - iMotions". Imotions Publish. 2019-04-01. Retrieved 30 June 2021.
- ^ Orban de Xivry, Jean-Jacques; Lefèvre, Philippe (2007-10-01). "Saccades and pursuit: two outcomes of a single sensorimotor process: Saccades and smooth pursuit eye movements". The Journal of Physiology. 584 (1): 11–23. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2007.139881. PMC 2277072. PMID 17690138.
- ^ Newsome, W. T; Wurtz, R. H; Dürsteler, M. R; Mikami, A (1985). "Deficits in visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey". The Journal of Neuroscience. 5 (3): 825–40. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.05-03-00825.1985. PMC 6565029. PMID 3973698.
- ^ Tian, J. R; Lynch, J. C (1996). "Corticocortical input to the smooth and saccadic eye movement subregions of the frontal eye field in Cebus monkeys". Journal of Neurophysiology. 76 (4): 2754–71. doi:10.1152/jn.1996.76.4.2754. PMID 8899643.
- ^ Krauzlis, R. J (2003). "Neuronal activity in the rostral superior colliculus related to the initiation of pursuit and saccadic eye movements". The Journal of Neuroscience. 23 (10): 4333–44. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-10-04333.2003. PMC 6741111. PMID 12764122.
- ^ Leigh, RJ; Zee, DS (2006). The Neurology of Eye Movements (4th ed.). Oxford University Press. pp. 209–11.
- ^ Coltz, J. D; Johnson, M. T; Ebner, T. J (2000). "Population code for tracking velocity based on cerebellar Purkinje cell simple spike firing in monkeys". Neuroscience Letters. 296 (1): 1–4. doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(00)01571-8. PMID 11099819. S2CID 40363291.
- ^ Krauzlis, R. J; Lisberger, S. G (1994). "Temporal properties of visual motion signals for the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys". Journal of Neurophysiology. 72 (1): 150–62. doi:10.1152/jn.1994.72.1.150. PMID 7965001.
- ^ Khurana, Beena; Kowler, Eileen (1987). "Shared attentional control of smooth eye movement and perception". Vision Research. 27 (9): 1603–18. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(87)90168-4. PMID 3445492. S2CID 32373643.
- ^ Souto, D; Kerzel, D (2008). "Dynamics of attention during the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements". Journal of Vision. 8 (14): 3.1–16. doi:10.1167/8.14.3. PMID 19146304.
- ^ a b Joiner, Wilsaan M; Shelhamer, Mark (2006). "Pursuit and saccadic tracking exhibit a similar dependence on movement preparation time". Experimental Brain Research. 173 (4): 572–86. doi:10.1007/s00221-006-0400-3. PMID 16550393. S2CID 19126627.
- ^ Krauzlis, Richard J (2016). "The Control of Voluntary Eye Movements: New Perspectives". The Neuroscientist. 11 (2): 124–37. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.135.8577. doi:10.1177/1073858404271196. PMID 15746381. S2CID 1439113.
- ^ a b Barnes, G.R (2008). "Cognitive processes involved in smooth pursuit eye movements". Brain and Cognition. 68 (3): 309–26. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.020. PMID 18848744. S2CID 34777376.
- ^ Berryhill, Marian E; Chiu, Tanya; Hughes, Howard C (2006). "Smooth Pursuit of Nonvisual Motion". Journal of Neurophysiology. 96 (1): 461–5. doi:10.1152/jn.00152.2006. PMID 16672304.
- ^ "Sensory Reception: Human Vision: Structure and function of the Human Eye" vol. 27, p. 179 Encyclopædia Britannica, 1987
- ^ Miles, F. A; Kawano, K; Optican, L. M (1986). "Short-latency ocular following responses of monkey. I. Dependence on temporospatial properties of visual input". Journal of Neurophysiology. 56 (5): 1321–54. doi:10.1152/jn.1986.56.5.1321. PMID 3794772.
- ^ Ilg, Uwe J (1997). "Slow eye movements". Progress in Neurobiology. 53 (3): 293–329. doi:10.1016/S0301-0082(97)00039-7. PMID 9364615. S2CID 39852528.
- ^ Krauzlis, Richard J (2004). "Recasting the Smooth Pursuit Eye Movement System". Journal of Neurophysiology. 91 (2): 591–603. doi:10.1152/jn.00801.2003. PMID 14762145.
- ^ Hong, L. Elliot; Tagamets, Malle; Avila, Matthew; Wonodi, Ikwunga; Holcomb, Henry; Thaker, Gunvant K (2005). "Specific motion processing pathway deficit during eye tracking in schizophrenia: A performance-matched functional magnetic resonance imaging study". Biological Psychiatry. 57 (7): 726–32. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.015. PMID 15820229. S2CID 20560856.
- ^ Avila, Matthew T; Hong, L. Elliot; Moates, Amanda; Turano, Kathleen A; Thaker, Gunvant K (2006). "Role of Anticipation in Schizophrenia-Related Pursuit Initiation Deficits". Journal of Neurophysiology. 95 (2): 593–601. doi:10.1152/jn.00369.2005. PMID 16267121.
- ^ Takarae, Y (2004). "Pursuit eye movement deficits in autism". Brain. 127 (12): 2584–94. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.580.5909. doi:10.1093/brain/awh307. PMID 15509622.
- ^ Cerbone, A; Sautter, F. J; Manguno-Mire, G; Evans, W. E; Tomlin, H; Schwartz, B; Myers, L (2003). "Differences in smooth pursuit eye movement between posttraumatic stress disorder with secondary psychotic symptoms and schizophrenia". Schizophrenia Research. 63 (1–2): 59–62. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(02)00341-9. PMID 12892858. S2CID 34234246.
- ^ Irwin, Harvey J; Green, Melissa J; Marsh, Pamela J (2016). "Dysfunction in Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements and History of Childhood Trauma". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 89 (3 Pt 2): 1230–6. doi:10.2466/pms.1999.89.3f.1230. PMID 10710773. S2CID 24180179.
- ^ Strand-Brodd, Katarina; Ewald, Uwe; Grönqvist, Helena; Holmström, Gerd; Strömberg, Bo; Grönqvist, Erik; von Hofsten, Claes; Rosander, Kerstin (2011). "Development of smooth pursuit eye movements in very preterm infants: 1. General aspects". Acta Paediatrica. 100 (7): 983–91. doi:10.1111/j.1651-2227.2011.02218.x. PMC 3123744. PMID 21332783.
- ^ Kaul, Ylva Fredriksson; Rosander, Kerstin; von Hofsten, Claes; Brodd, Katarina Strand; Holmström, Gerd; Kaul, Alexander; Böhm, Birgitta; Hellström-Westas, Lena (2016). "Visual tracking in very preterm infants at 4 mo predicts neurodevelopment at 3 y of age". Pediatric Research. 80 (1): 35–42. doi:10.1038/pr.2016.37. PMID 27027722.
Further reading
editExternal links
edit- Pursuit,+Smooth at the U.S. National Library of Medicine Medical Subject Headings (MeSH)