Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2011 Aug 1;34(8):987-95.
doi: 10.5665/SLEEP.1148.

Associations between sleep-wake consolidation and language development in early childhood: a longitudinal twin study

Affiliations

Associations between sleep-wake consolidation and language development in early childhood: a longitudinal twin study

Ginette Dionne et al. Sleep. .

Abstract

Study objectives: The objectives were (1) to assess associations between sleep consolidation at 6, 18 and 30 months and language skills at 18, 30, and 60 months; and (2) to investigate the genetic/environmental etiology of these associations.

Design: Longitudinal study of a population-based twin cohort.

Participants: 1029 twins from the Quebec Newborn Twin Study.

Measurements and results: Sleep consolidation was derived from parental reports of day/night consecutive sleeping durations. Language skills were assessed with the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory at 18 and 30 months and the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test at 60 months. The day/night sleep ratio decreased significantly from 6 to 30 months. The 6- and 18-month ratios were negatively correlated with subsequent language skills. Children with language delays at 60 months had less mature sleep consolidation at both 6 and 18 months than children without delays and those with transient early delays. Genetic and regression analyses revealed that the sleep ratio at 6 months was highly heritable (64%) and predicted 18-month (B = -0.06) and 30-month language (B = -0.11) mainly through additive genetic influences (R(Gs) = 0.32 and 0.33, respectively). By contrast, the sleep ratio at 18 months was mainly due to shared environment influences (58%) and predicted 60-month language (B = -0.08) through shared environment influences (R(Cs) = 0.24).

Conclusions: Poor sleep consolidation during the first 2 years of life may be a risk factor for language learning, whereas good sleep consolidation may foster language learning through successive genetic and environmental influences.

Keywords: Sleep-wake consolidation; early childhood; genetics; language.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Path diagram depicting the genetic and environmental model of the correlation between sleep consolidation and language. A, C, and E refer to additive genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental influences, respectively. RG, RC, and RE refer to the genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental correlations derived from covariance estimates.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Changes in means and standard deviations (SD) for the ratio of consecutive daytime sleep/consecutive nighttime sleep (CDS/CNS) between 6, 18 and 30 months of age adjusted for gestation duration; F2,533 = 280.27, P < 0.001. N = 977 at 6 months, 881 at 18 months, and 797 at 30 months.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Longitudinal language groups as a function of the ratio of consecutive daytime sleep/consecutive nighttime sleep (CDS/CNS) at 6 and 18 months. Children with persistent or late language delays (■ N = 140) differed significantly from children without language delays (☐ N = 385) and from children with transient delays (formula image N = 93) on both the 6- and 18-month sleep ratio. Sleep ratio was adjusted for gestation duration corrected age.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Beckwith L, Parmelee AH., Jr EEG patterns of preterm infants, home environment, and later IQ. Child Dev. 1986;57:777–89. - PubMed
    1. Anders TF, Keener MA. Developmental course of nighttime sleep-wake patterns in full-term and premature infants during the first year of life: I. Sleep. 1985;8:173–92. - PubMed
    1. Borghese IF, Minard KL, Thoman EB. Sleep rhythmicity in premature infants: implications for developmental status. Sleep. 1995;18:523–30. - PubMed
    1. Gertner S, Greenbaum CW, Sadeh A, Dolfin Z, Sirota L, Ben-Nun Y. Sleep-wake patterns in preterm infants and 6 month's home environment: implications for early cognitive development. Early Hum Dev. 2002;68:93–102. - PubMed
    1. Dahl RE. The impact of inadequate sleep on children's daytime cognitive function. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 1996;3:44–50. - PubMed

Publication types

  NODES
Association 6
twitter 2